Risk: Exposure to Disease



Risk: Exposure to Disease






From the study of the characteristics of persons who later develop coronary heart disease (CHD) and comparison with the characteristics of those who remain free of this disease, it is possible many years before any overt symptoms or signs become manifest… to put together a profile of those persons in whom there is a high risk of developing CHD … It has seldom been possible in noninfectious disease to identify such highly susceptible individuals years before the development of disease.

—Thomas Dawber and William Kannel 1961




STUDIES OF RISK

This chapter describes how investigators obtain estimates of risk by observing the relationship between exposure to possible risk factors and the subsequent incidence of disease. It describes methods used to determine risk by following groups into the future and also discusses several ways of comparing risks as they affect individuals and populations. Chapter 6 describes methods of studying risk by looking backward in time.

The most powerful way to determine whether exposure to a potential risk factor results in an increased risk of disease is to conduct an experiment in which the researcher determines who is exposed. People currently without disease are divided into groups of equal susceptibility to the disease in question. One group is exposed to the purported risk factor and the other is not, but the groups otherwise are treated the same. Later, any difference in observed rates of disease in the groups can be attributed to the risk factor. Experiments are discussed in Chapter 9.


When Experiments Are Not Possible or Ethical

The effects of most risk factors in humans cannot be studied with experimental studies. Consider some of the risk questions that concern us today: Are inactive people at increased risk for cardiovascular disease, everything else being equal? Do cellular phones cause brain cancer? Does obesity increase the risk of cancer? For such questions, it is usually not possible to conduct an experiment. First, it would be unethical
to impose possible risk factors on a group of healthy people for the purposes of scientific research. Second, most people would balk at having their diets and behaviors constrained by others for long periods of time. Finally, the experiment would have to go on for many years, which is difficult and expensive. As a result, it is usually necessary to study risk in less obtrusive ways.

Clinical studies in which the researcher gathers data by simply observing events as they happen, without playing an active part in what takes place, are called observational studies. Most studies of risk are observational studies and are either cohort studies, described in the rest of this chapter, or case-control studies, described in Chapter 6.


Cohorts

As defined in Chapter 2, the term cohort is used to describe a group of people who have something in common when they are first assembled and who are then observed for a period of time to see what happens to them. Table 5.1 lists some of the ways in which cohorts are used in clinical research. Whatever members of a cohort have in common, observations of them should fulfill three criteria if the observations are to provide sound information about risk of disease.








Table 5.1 Cohorts and Their Purposes































Characteristic in Common


To Assess Effect of


Example


Age


Age


Life expectancy for people age 70 (regardless of birth date)


Date of birth


Calendar time


Tuberculosis rates for people born in 1930


Exposure


Risk factor


Lung cancer in people who smoke


Disease


Prognosis


Survival rate for patients with brain cancer


Therapeutic intervention


Treatment


Improvement in survival for patients with Hodgkin lymphoma given combination chemotherapy


Preventive intervention


Prevention


Reduction in incidence of pneumonia after pneumococcal vaccination




  • They do not have the disease (or outcome) in question at the time they are assembled.


  • They should be observed over a meaningful period of time in the natural history of the disease in question so that there will be sufficient time for the risk to be expressed. For example, if one wanted to learn whether neck irradiation during childhood results in thyroid neoplasms, a 5-year follow-up would not be a fair test of this hypothesis, because the usual time period between radiation exposure and the onset of disease is considerably longer.


  • All members of the cohort should be observed over the full period of follow-up or methods must be used to account for dropouts. To the extent that people drop out of the study and their reasons for dropping out are related in some way to the outcome, the information provided by an incomplete cohort can misrepresent the true state of affairs.


Cohort Studies

The basic design of a cohort study is illustrated in Figure 5.1. A group of people (a cohort) is assembled, none of whom has experienced the outcome of interest, but all of whom could experience it. (For example, in a study of risk factors for endometrial cancer, each member of the cohort should have an intact uterus.) Upon entry into the study, people in the cohort are classified according to those characteristics (possible risk factors) that might be related to outcome. For each possible risk factor, members of the cohort are classified either as exposed (i.e., possessing the factor in question, such as hypertension) or unexposed. All the members of the cohort are then observed over time to see which of them experience the outcome, say, cardiovascular disease, and the rates of the outcome events are compared in the exposed and unexposed groups. It is then possible to see whether potential risk factors are related to subsequent outcome events. Other names for cohort studies are incidence studies, which emphasize that patients are followed over time; prospective studies, which imply the forward direction in which the patients are pursued; and longitudinal studies, which call attention to the basic measure of new disease events over time.

The following is a description of a classic cohort study that has made important contributions to our understanding of cardiovascular disease risk factors and to modern methods of conducting cohort studies.







Figure 5.1 ▪ Design of a cohort study of risk. Persons without disease are divided into two groups—those exposed to a risk factor and those not exposed. Both groups are followed over time to determine what proportion of each group develops disease.



Prospective and Historical Cohort Studies

Cohort studies can be conducted in two ways (Fig. 5.2). The cohort can be assembled in the present and followed into the future (a prospective cohort study), or it can be identified from past records and followed forward from that time up to the present (a retrospective cohort study or a historical cohort study). The Framingham Study an example of a prospective cohort study. Useful retrospective cohort studies are appearing increasingly in the medical literature because of the availability of large computerized medical databases.


Prospective Cohort Studies

Prospective cohort studies can assess purported risk factors not usually captured in medical records, including many health behaviors, educational level, and socioeconomic status, which have been found to have important health effects. When the study is planned before data are collected, researchers can be sure to collect information about possible confounders. Finally, all the information in a prospective cohort study can be collected in a standardized manner that decreases measurement bias.







Figure 5.2 ▪ Retrospective and prospective cohort studies. Prospective cohorts are assembled in the present and followed forward into the future. In contrast, retrospective cohorts are made by going back into the past and assembling the cohort, for example, from medical records, then following the group forward to the present.



Historical Cohort Studies Using Medical Databases

Historical cohort studies can take advantage of computerized medical databases and population registries that are used primarily for patient care or to track population health. The major advantages of historical cohort studies over classical prospective cohort studies are that they take less time, are less expensive, and are much easier to do. However, they cannot undertake studies of factors not recorded in computerized databases, so patients’ lifestyle, social standing, education, and other important health determinants usually cannot be included in the studies. Also, information in many databases, especially medical care information, is not collected in a standardized manner, leading to the possibility of bias in results. Large computerized databases are particularly useful for studying possible risk factors and health outcomes that are likely to be recorded in medical databases in somewhat standard ways, such as diagnoses and treatments.





Advantages and Disadvantages of Cohort Studies

Well-conducted cohort studies of risk, regardless of type, are the best available substitutes for a true experiment when experimentation is not possible. They follow the same logic as a clinical trial and allow measurement of exposure to a possible risk factor while avoiding any possibility of bias that might occur if exposure were determined after the outcome was already known. The most important scientific disadvantage of cohort studies (in fact, all observational studies) is that they are subject to a great many more potential biases than are experiments. People who are exposed to a certain risk factor in the natural course of events are likely to differ in a great many ways from a comparison group of people not exposed to the factor. If some of these other differences are also related to the disease in question, they could confound any association observed between the putative risk factor and the disease.

The uses, strengths, and limitations of the different types of cohort studies are summarized in Table 5.2. Several of the advantages and disadvantages apply regardless of type. However, the potential
for difficulties with the quality of data is different for the three. In prospective studies, data can be collected specifically for the purposes of the study and with full anticipation of what is needed. It is thereby possible to avoid measurement biases and some of the confounders that might undermine the accuracy of the results. However, data for historical cohorts are usually gathered for other purposes—often as part of medical records for patient care. Except for carefully selected questions, such as the relationship between vaccination and autism, the data in historical cohort studies may not be of sufficient quality for rigorous research.








Table 5.2 Advantages and Disadvantages of Cohort Studies





















































Advantages


Disadvantages


All Cohort Study Types


The only way of establishing incidence (i.e., absolute risk) directly


Susceptible to confounding and other biases


Follows the same logic as the clinical question: If persons are exposed, do they get the disease?



Exposure can be elicited without the bias that might occur if outcome were known before documentation of exposure



Can assess the relationship between exposure and many diseases



Prospective Cohort Studies


Can study a wide range of possible risk factors


Inefficient because many more subjects must be enrolled than experience the event of interest; therefore, cannot be used for rare diseases


Can collect lifestyle and demographic data not available in most medical records


Expensive because of resources necessary to study many people over time


Can set up standardized ways of measuring exposure and degree of exposure to risk factors


Results not available for a long time



Assesses the relationship between disease and exposure to only relatively few factors (i.e., those recorded at the outset of the study)


Retrospective (Historical) Cohort Studies


More efficient than prospective cohort studies because data have already been collected for another purpose (i.e., during patient care or for a registry)


Range of possible risk factors that can be studied is narrower than that possible with prospective cohort studies


Cheaper than prospective cohort studies because resources not necessary to follow many people over time


Cannot examine patient characteristics not available in the data set used


Faster than prospective cohort studies because patient outcomes have already occurred


Measurement of exposure and degree of exposure may not be standardized


Case-cohort Studies


All advantages of retrospective cohort studies apply


All disadvantages of retrospective cohort studies apply


Even more efficient than retrospective cohort studies because only a sample of unexposed group is analyzed


Difficult for readers to understand weighting procedures used in the analysis


Prospective cohort studies can also collect data on lifestyle and other characteristics that might influence the results, and they can do so in standard ways. Many of these characteristics are not routinely available in retrospective and case-cohort studies, and those that are usually are not collected in standard ways.


The principal disadvantage of prospective cohort studies is that when the outcome is infrequent, which is usually so in studies of risk, a large number of people must be entered in a study and remain under observation for a long time before results are available. Having to measure exposure in many people and then follow them for years is inefficient when few ultimately develop the disease. For example, the Framingham Study of cardiovascular disease (the most common cause of death in America) was the largest study of its kind when it began. Nevertheless, more than 5,000 people had to be followed for several years before the first, preliminary conclusions could be published. Only 5% of the people had experienced a coronary event during the first 8 years. Retrospective and case-cohort studies get around the problem of time but often sacrifice access to important and standardized data.

Another problem with prospective cohort studies results from the people under study usually being “free living” and not under the control of researchers. A great deal of effort and money must be expended to keep track of them. Prospective cohort studies of risk, therefore, are expensive, usually costing many millions, sometimes hundreds of millions, of dollars.

Because of the time and money required for prospective cohort studies, this approach cannot be used for all clinical questions about risk, which was a major reason for efforts to find more efficient, yet dependable, ways of assessing risk, such as retrospective and case-cohort designs. Another method, case-control studies, is discussed in Chapter 6.








Table 5.3 Measures of Effect





























Expression


Question


Definitiona


Absolute risk


What is the incidence of disease in a group initially free of the condition?


image


Attributable risk (risk difference)


What is the incidence of disease attributable to exposure?


image


Relative risk (risk ratio)


How many times more likely are exposed persons to become diseased, relative to non-exposed persons?


image


Population-attributable risk


What is the incidence of disease in a population, associated with the prevalence of a risk factor?


image


Population-attributable fraction


What fraction of the disease in a population is attributable to exposure to a risk factor?


image


a Where IE = incidence in exposed persons; IĒ = incidence in non-exposed persons; P = prevalence of exposure to a risk factor; and IT = total incidence of disease in a population.



WAYS TO EXPRESS AND COMPARE RISK

The basic expression of risk is incidence, which is defined in Chapter 2 as the number of new cases of disease arising during a given period of time in a defined population that is initially free of the condition. In cohort studies, the incidence of disease is compared in two or more groups that differ in exposure to a possible risk factor. To compare risks, several measures of the association between exposure and disease, called measures of effect, are commonly used. These measures represent different concepts of risk, elicit different impressions of the magnitude of a risk, and are used for different purposes. Four measures of effect are discussed in the following text. Table 5.3 summarizes the four, along with absolute risk, and Table 5.4 demonstrates their use with the risk of lung cancer among smokers and non-smokers.


Absolute Risk

Absolute risk is the probability of an event in a population under study. Its value is the same as that for incidence, and the terms are often used interchangeably. Absolute risk is the best way for individual patients and clinicians to understand how risk factors may affect their lives. Thus, as Table 5.4 shows, although smoking greatly increases the chances of dying from lung cancer, among smokers the absolute risk of dying from lung cancer each year in the population studied was 341.3 per
100,000 (3 to 4 lung cancer deaths per 1,000 smokers per year).






Table 5.4 Calculating Measures of Effect: Cigarette Smoking and Death from Lung Cancer in Mena

Only gold members can continue reading. Log In or Register to continue

Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

Jul 5, 2016 | Posted by in INFECTIOUS DISEASE | Comments Off on Risk: Exposure to Disease

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access